When I was a child, my favorite book was 101 Things To Do on a Rainy Day.
The resource was full of all kinds of neat suggestions of games and activities
that house-bound kids could do when the weather turned nasty. Through
easy-to-follow pictures and brief, clearly written descriptions the book
showed one how to make such things as rubber band-propelled boats and
live-in villages with blanket-covered card tables. I can remember consulting
pages in that manual into my adolescent years even after the book's binding
had given up holding the pages in place.
As
I think back on those days, I'm struck at how dependent I was on 101 Things
To Do on a Rainy Day to get me through stormy days. It was a "cookbook"
of fun activities. All I had to do was follow the directions and pictures
in the text to end up with a new invention, the reward of my effort. Oh,
I'm sure some kids discovered different ways of working with the activities
than those suggested, and some may even have found new rewards by inventing
thing that were not in the text after being primed by the "cookbook" descriptions.
But most kids just had fun with the activities as they were described
in the 315 pages of the manual.
I still have that rainy day resource book. It's stuck away in a molding
box full of ancient papers and texts in my attic or corner of my office
at the college. I had just begun to search my bookshelves when a student
appeared at the open door to my office. She had missed the last few of
my science classes of the semester and wanted to know the page numbers
in her text that corresponded with the material covered in the classes.
I asked her if she wanted to go over the material, but she didn't think
she needed to because the textbook was full of charts, pictures and clear
descriptions of the content I present in class. She was sure she could
get the information she needed from the book. I pulled the text from the
shelf next to my desk, thumbed to the table of contents in the front,
and told her the chapters and page numbers we had covered while she was
absent. She thanked me for my help and left to join her two friends waiting
for her in the hall.
I
sat there for a minute or two thinking about what the student had said.
Could she really get everything she needed to know directly out of the
text? Don't I play a larger role in a student's learning than disseminating
information that can just as easily be gotten from the textbook? I look
through the brightly colored pages of the resource with its slick graphics
and splendid photographs. It was all there! All of the content a student
needed to know to be rewarded with a passing grade in my course. All I
did in class was to present the same content in the manual using overheads
and an occasional slide or two. I was just an extension of the textbook
itself. Amazed, I found myself looking at a science version of my childhood
book, 101 Things To Do on a Rainy Day.
With
this came the realization that the instructional method most often employed
by professors to teach science in today's colleges and universities is
the same as the one I used with my rainy-day source book. For many of
our courses, all a student needs to do is to recite the directions in
the text or lecture on a test to end up with the reward of passing our
courses. Shouldn't professors excite students to think beyond the descriptions
in their texts? Shouldn't we encourage students to look critically at
the content or extend their thinking to other activities? Are we stressing
the facts in the course so much that students are missing the overall
picture?
I
think one of the biggest fallacies in education today is the belief that
content recitation and regurgitation confirms content comprehension and
understanding. If a student writes a paper on the course material and
passes a few tests about the subject, we reward the student with a passing
grade for our course. The remarkable thing is that dozens of studies have
found that most college students retain grand misconceptions and limited
understanding about the subject after they have successfully passed the
course, sometimes with an A+ (Kyle & Shyrnansky, 1989; Perkins, 19C3;
Lorsbach, Tobin, Briscoe, & LaMaster, 1995).
In
an attempt to explain why this is occurring, educational theorists have
turned their attention to how a person actually gains new knowledge. This
initiative has led many academics to question the behaviorist concept
of learning, the "tried and true" philosophy of education that for decades
formed the backbone of science instruction at all levels.
The
behavioral model of science education holds that the teacher's job is
to present stimuli and reinforcements that require students to respond
in specific ways. For years, people viewed science as a means of finding
truth, and truth was based on the factual content in journals and books.
Because of this, students were taught to view science objectively rather
than with imagination and understanding. As a result, teachers planned
science lessons based on the facts they found in textbooks and other resources
and evaluated students on their ability to recount the information. When
the evaluation of student knowledge is based on rote memorization and
reiteration of content, most students do quite well. If it is the objective
of science teaching to have students replicate information, then the behavioral
belief of teaching works beautifully. If, however, the goal of science
teaching is to develop students who understand what the factual content
is actually about and are able to apply this new knowledge to other situations,
then the behavioral model of teaching fails miserably (Yaser, 1991).
Realizing that the old teaching methods are not producing true understanding,
educators are beginning to examine what is really involved in achieving
knowledge. This has led to the understanding that knowledge isn't something
that lies outside one's body. Rather, conceptualization resides within
a person. True knowledge isn't gained by students passively reading paragraphs
in a text, looking at charts and pictures, watching videos or television,
or hearing information recited by presenters. Students must actively pursue
thinking. To acquire new knowledge, they must expend energy to allow the
new information to interact with already perceived notions.
If
one accepts this notion of knowledge acquisition, he or she will appreciate
the learning philosophy called constructivism. Constructivists believe
that the learner uses mental energy to construct new knowledge on the
foundations of prior intellect (Glasersfeld, 1989).
Once
students properly assimilate new knowledge, they reach real understanding
of the issues. When this occurs, the enlightened person can easily explain
what he or she has learned to others. In fact, verbal and written exchanges
with colleagues are important in learning. By attempting to explain what
one knows about a subject to someone else, the explainer tests the fit
of his or her understanding of the material (Lorsbach & Tobin, 1993).
Similarly, while tying to understand what someone is saying, active listeners
question and challenge their own understanding and try to fit the information
onto their already established cognitive foundations.
Constructivists
believe that in the acquisition of knowledge, mental energies are expended
by both the deliverer and the receiver. In a traditional, teacher directed
classroom, mental energy is generally expended by the teacher (the deliverer)
for the entire lesson. However, the expenditure of cognitive energy by
the student (the receiver) that enables him or her to build the new knowledge
on the old foundations rarely exceeds 10 to 12 straight minutes.
In a constructivist-based, student-centered classroom, the students' cognitive
energy continues to take place for most of the class period. The teacher's
mental energy also remains high throughout the lesson as he or she moves
about assisting groups of students in their attempt to construct new knowledge.
This conceptually directed vitality in students leads to higher interest
and enthusiasm for the discipline. The increased student fascination for
the subject reinforces the instructor's efforts and leads to more productive
and enjoyable teaching. The end result is a wholesome learning environment
that promotes curiosity, critical thinking, and lasting retention of knowledge.
LITERATURE
CITED
|