Reasons for science instruction are usually stated in terms of cognitive
objectives: to help children develop the ability to solve problems; to
engage in the processes of inquiry, exploration and discovery; to develop
skills in observation, verification, hypothesizing and reasoning; and
to gain scientific knowledge and information. When we consider Einstein's
thoughts on science instruction, however, it is clear that he believed
the very foundations of science lie in affect (emotion). He captures a
side of science that resides in the affective realm of curiosity and enjoyment.
Einstein, the consummate intellectual, admonished educators to promote
the affective side of science or else risk strangling the scientific process.
A
case for the "affective side of science instruction" begins with our conceptualization
of learning. With the advent of Skinner behaviorism, learning theory has
been dominated by "what can be observed" in learning. Since emotion is
more difficult to observe and document, it has been either ignored or
placed on the periphery of the learning process. Piaget (1967), however,
wisely pulls human learning into a "whole" construct of both affect and
cognition. This is contrary to the traditional paradigm of learning, which
focuses primarily on the cognitive side. Piaget, known for his cognitive
learning theory, does not promote cognition in absence of affect. Instead,
he envisions learning as one interactive process of affect and cognition.
Cognition and affect are co-constructed by constant interaction between
the two; the two cannot be considered separately in the learning process
(Bearison & Zimiles, 1986; Piaget, 1967).
Within
this interactive process, emotions direct us to relate to our environment.
Zimiles (1981) suggests that the affect system is not merely a necessary
condition for learning, an independent driving force, but rather an interactive
element that, in addition to setting off the learning process, contributes
to its scope and character by steering and monitoring the cognitive performance.
(p. 52)
Interest
directs and organizes the cognitive activity of assimilation (Langsdorf,
Izard, Rayias & Hembree, 1983). Bearison and Zimiles (1986) suggest that
"without affect there would be no interest, no need, no motivation; and
consequently ... there would be no intelligence" (p. 3). From a Piagetian
point of view, the affective side of assimilation is interest, whereas
the cognitive side is understanding (Langsdorf et al., 1983; Piaget, 1967).
Carson
(1960) notes that "first emotion is aroused; the emotions serve as a catalyst
to seek knowledge. Once found, the information has lasting meaning" (p.
45). When, however, "emotions (associated with an event) are muted, memories
of the event are less enduring" (Restak, 1979, p. 194). That is, we remember
what we understand, and our ability to grasp understanding depends on
our motivation and interest.
Harlan
(1988) characterizes science as the human desire to understand the world
and how it is organized. It is curiosity and the desire to understand
that motivate individuals to seek knowledge. Thus, scientific genius does
not develop from prescribed lessons with defined objectives; it occurs
when curiosity and passion for discovery are cultivated.
Let us consider, for example, the words of Richard Feynman, Nobel Laureate
in physics. He combined desire and enjoyment as the motivation for his
pursuit of the science of physics:
Why
did I enjoy doing it [physics]? I used to play with it. I used to do whatever
I felt like doing. It didn't have to do with whether it was important
for the development of nuclear physics, but instead whether it was interesting
and amusing for me to play with ... I'd invent things and play with things
for my own entertainment ... [Once] I was in the cafeteria and some guy,
fooling around, throws me a plate in the air. As the plate went up in
the air, I saw it wobble, and I noticed the red medallion of Cornell [University]
on the plate going around. It was pretty obvious to me that the medallion
went around faster than the wobbling.
I
had nothing to do, so I started to figure out the motion of the rotating
plate. I discovered that when the angle is very slight, the medallion
rotates twice as fast as the wobble rate: two to one. It came out as a
complicated equation! I don't remember how I did it, but I ultimately
worked out what the motion of the mass particles is, and how all the accelerations
balance to make it come out two to one. I still remember Hans Bethe saying
to me, "Hey, Feynman. That's pretty interesting, but what's the importance
of it? Why are you doing it?"
Hah,
I say. There's no importance whatsoever. I'm just doing it for the fun
of it. His reaction didn't discourage me. I had made up my mind. I was
going to enjoy physics and do whatever I liked ... The diagrams and the
whole business that I got the Nobel Prize for came from that messing around
with the wobbling plate. (Feynman, 1985, p. 173-174)
Feynman's curiosity, desire and enjoyment led him to seek knowledge in
the realm of physics (Harlan, 1988). Without affect, Feynman would not
have engaged in the scientific process of discovery that ultimately won
him the Nobel Prize. This is not an obscure example of one scientist's
adventure into new knowledge; rather, Feynman's experience is reflective
of the "whole" process of learning, the interaction of affect and cognition.
In
addressing methods of science instruction, we must provide for learning
experiences that include both affect and cognition. The classroom teacher
should consider three critical areas:
*
The Affective Environment. Providing an enriched environment that encourages
hands-on experiences (i.e., centers, projects) is a necessary component
of active, cognitive learning. The knowledgeable teacher, however, must
also establish the affective environment, which interacts with cognitive
learning.
First,
the affective environment must be "risk-free" so that children have the
opportunity to explore, experiment and solve problems without the fear
of failure. Play creates a natural risk-free environment. Opportunities
for free-play with science materials promote a child's curiosity and willingness
to consider varying options (Henniger, 1988). Wassermann (1992) notes
that In play, there is no fear of failure, because there is no failure.
Failure occurs when children have not measured up to another's preconceived
notion of what they should have done. No standards of right and wrong
are articulated in play, and the absence of such standards is what allows
for innovation. (p. 135)
Second,
the affective environment is enjoyable. As Feynman noted, he "enjoyed"
playing with physics problems. Affective environments that nurture joy
allow children to create, invent and pursue solutions.
Third,
the affective environment eliminates the stress of time constraints. Children
must be able to play freely with science materials, without a strict time
frame in which to finish, complete or draw conclusions. The time frame
must be adequate so that children feel the freedom to explore different
solutions or create multiple scenarios.
Fourth,
the affective environment provides choice, which is an expression of the
affect interest, and is an integral part of the "desire to know." Children
will make their own discoveries during free-play with their chosen science
materials and they will construct their own knowledge of a phenomenon.
Also, through choice, children feel ownership of the process, rather than
feeling coerced or obliged to explore (Einstein, 1954).
*
The Affective Teacher. Children are more likely to imitate behaviors and
attitudes that they perceive as pleasurable and beneficial. Thus, children's
attitudes are greatly shaped by how a teacher engages in the science process.
It is the responsibility of the teacher to model the affective realm of
curiosity, excitement and enjoyment, as well as a desire to know. Her
feelings of wonder and joy will, in turn, generate interest in the scientific
process of discovery.
Rivkin
(1992) notes that "laughing, squealing, joyously shared science is a goal
worth seeking for yourself and for children. People learn best what they
enjoy" (p. 4). The affective teacher should also value and support children
in their own explorations, demonstrating that she is comfortable with
"playing" and "messing around" with scientific materials. If the teacher
is overly concerned with following exact procedures, keeping materials
clean or orderly, and conducting a quiet classroom, her feelings will
impede the process of discovery. Children, who naturally embrace the science
adventure, often can be conditioned over time to no longer care, or want
to know. The non-affective teacher can strangle the "curiosity of inquiry,"
the "joy of seeing and searching" (Einstein, 1954).
* The Affective Child. A teacher should understand and support the affect
of the child by realizing that "emotions are constantly at work in the
functioning of thought" (Izard, 1986). A child's affect will positively
or negatively reinforce his desire to know.
First,
a teacher should support a child's autonomy. Autonomous children freely
explore their interests in self-directed learning, while children who
are controlled by adults are less likely to follow through on their curiosity,
or to rely on their own problem-solving efforts.
Second,
a teacher should understand the role of self-confidence and self-esteem.
The self-confident child will pursue a problem, whereas an insecure child
will not. A child with low self-confidence may enter into a state of "learned
helplessness," thus withdrawing from an experience or problem-solving
situation.
Children
with positive self-esteem are risk-takers, because they see themselves
as competent individuals. They are more apt to explore and engage in problem-solving
experiences, are less anxious, concentrate more, spend more time on task
because of enjoyment, attend to critical elements of a problem, are more
spontaneous and are more apt to pursue and generate multiple solutions
to problems (Fenn & Iwanicki, 1986; Flynn, 1984). Science-related self-esteem
should result in a child stating, "I am a scientist," rather than "I'm
no good at science."
Third,
a teacher should promote each child's success. Feelings of success or
failure can, respectively, facilitate or inhibit learning (Izard, 1986).
Children who expect success will seek out increasingly difficult problems,
because children naturally pursue problems they feel they can solve. A
child will have greater opportunity for success in free-play exploration
and open-ended science activities than when trying to solve an experiment
that only has one answer. Anxiety, fear of failure and learned helplessness
thrive in a science curriculum that only accepts one right answer, and
in which every experiment is devised by the teacher.
To
allow a child's affect to interact with cognition, the teacher should
provide him with greater freedom to explore, to choose his problems to
solve, to try new ways of doing things, to experience success and to have
sufficient time to let his mind and emotions enjoy the process of discovery.
In
conclusion, teachers must consider the innate passion to discover and
the desire to understand when designing science instruction. In an emotionally
secure and stimulating environment, children will freely express the passions
to discover and understand. We must ensure and support every child's affective
development along with his cognitive development. We cannot address the
cognitive side of science without wisely promoting the affective side
as well. If not, we are making a truly grave mistake (Einstein, 1954).
Added material
REFERENCES
|
Bearison, D. J., & Zimiles, H. (1986). Developmental prospectives on thought
and emotion: An introduction. In D. J. Bearison & H. Zimiles (Eds.), Thought
and emotion (pp. 1-10). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carson,
R. (1960). A sense of wonder. New York: Harper and Row.
Einstein,
A. (1954). Ideas and opinions. New York: Crown.
Fenn,
L. M., & Iwanicki, E. F. (1986). An investigation of the relationship
between student affective characteristics and student achievement within
more and less affective school settings. Journal of Research and Development
in Education, 19(4), 10-18.
Feynman,
R. (1985). Surely you're joking Mr. Feynman. New York: Norton.
Flynn,
T. M. (1984). Affective characteristics that predict preschool achievement
in disadvantaged children. Early Childhood Development and Care, 16, 251-264.
Harlan, J. D. (1988). Science experiences for the early childhood years
(4th ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Henniger,
M. L. (1988). Learning mathematics and science through play. In J. P.
Bauch (Ed.), Early childhood education in the schools. Washington, DC:
National Education Association.
Izard,
C. (1986). Approaches to developmental research on emotion-cognition relationships.
In D. J. Bearison & H. Zimiles (Eds.), Thought and emotion (pp. 21-37).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Langsdorf,
P., Izard, D. E., Rayias, M., & Hembree, E. A. (1983). Interest expression,
visual fixation, and heart rate changes in 2- to 8-month-old infants.
Developmental Psychology, 19(3), 375-386.
Piaget,
J. (1967). Six psychological studies. New York: Vintage Books.
Restak,
R. (1979). The brain: The last frontier. New York: Doubleday.
Rivkin,
M. (1992). Science is a way of life. Young Children, 47(4), 4-8.
Wassermann, S. (1992). Serious play in the classroom. Childhood Education,
68, 133-139.
Zimiles, H. (1981). Cognitive-affective interaction: A concept that exceeds
the researcher's grasp. In E. K. Shapiro & E. Weber (Eds.), Cognitive
and affective growth: Developmental interaction (pp. 47-63). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
AUTHOR:
SANDRA
J. STONE AND KATHLEEN GLASCOTT
SOURCE:
Childhood Education v74 p102-4 Winter '97-'98 The magazine publisher is
the copyright holder of this article and it is reproduced with permission.WBN:
9701503906008
|